CBTp changes the brain’s wiring? Extraordinary claims, ordinary evidence

mechanical-2033446_1280

Cognitive Behavioural Therapy for Psychosis (CBTp) has negligible effects on psychotic symptoms in randomised controlled trials where raters are ‘blind’ (or masked) to what treatment the participants are receiving (Jauhar et al, 2014).

One of the few CBTp studies that has reported significant changes in psychotic symptoms using “masked” raters also reported parallel changes in brain function and connectivity (Kumari et al 2011; Mason et al 2015), although in a non-randomised trial. Now of course, if a treatment for psychotic symptoms works to reduce symptoms, then we would expect to see changes in brain function.

In the original study, Kumari et al, (2011) compared 22 people diagnosed with schizophrenia before and after they had received 16 sessions of CBTp with 16 patients who had received treatment-as-usual (TAU). All underwent Functional MRI (fMRI) while simply deciding if faces expressing fear, anger, happy or neutral expressions were male or female. Scanning revealed altered activation in several brain areas including the amygdala when viewing angry expressions and crucially, this activity correlated with symptom improvement following CBT. Such findings have been used to support the notion that CBTp changes brain activity, although of course, almost everything we do changes brain activity…including reading this sentence.

This latest study by Mason et al (2017) claims that the functional brain changes following 16 sessions of CBTp predict recovery in schizophrenia up to 8 years later – a bold assertion.

Almost everything we do changes brain activity, even looking at this picture of a kitten.

Almost everything we do changes brain activity, even looking at this picture of a kitten.

Methods

Although the abstract mentions 22 participants, (from the original Kumari et al study in 2011) the data are from 15 of their original patients diagnosed with ‘paranoid schizophrenia’, who were followed up 7-9 years later.

Data collected from the TAU control group from the earlier studies were not included in the analyses here; a serious weakness.

Electronic case notes were reviewed and monthly symptom levels recorded and then participants allocated to full/partial or no remission outcomes. The ratings were made by the paper’s first author and so, ratings were obviously made knowing that patients had received CBT. This underscores not only the issue of blind assessment (which is known to be a major uncontrolled inflator of the effects of apparent therapeutic success in CBTp research: Jauhar et al 2014), but again the necessity of having a control group; both for ratings and imaging data.

Participants themselves completed the Questionnaire about the Process of Recovery (QPR,31) measuring domains such as hope or empowerment; so, again unblinded (self)assessment. An fMRI was performed including assessment of regional brain activity during tasks measuring reactions to angry and happy faces as well as connectivity between regions of interest.

Multiple statistical tests were used; one-tailed (which increases the chance of finding a significant result) and given the small number of participants it is unclear if this was sufficiently considered when checking if these results were significant. A Bonferroni correction was said to be applied, which may mitigate this concern. It is unclear if the proposed analysis was pre-registered before the data was analysed, which would reduce concerns that “fishing” for positive results took place.

Results

Many tests were performed and only a few appeared to be statistically significant:

  1. Long-term psychotic symptoms were predicted by changes in prefrontal connections during prosocial facial affective processing
  2. Long-term affective symptoms were predicted by threat-related amygdalo-inferior parietal lobe connectivity
  3. Higher subjective ratings of recovery at long-term follow-up were predicted by greater increases in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex connectivity with amygdala

The authors’ three key results above are interpreted as positive; however none actually reach conventional levels of acceptable statistical significance (where p <=0.05; but are p=0.06 both in 1) and in 2); and p=.052 in 3, although the latter is reported as p=.05). Leaving aside debates about the use of the terms ‘marginally significant’ or ‘approaching significance’, all three results are nonsignificant.

Furthermore, some of the presented results are clearly driven by the influence of outlying data from single individuals. For example, the third claim that increased dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC); amygdala connectivity is associated with higher levels of subjective recovery. In this example, one person scores abnormally highly on the change in amygdala-DLPFC connectivity (see Figure 2 in the paper).

The same is true for the claimed increase in connectivity between dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and postcentral gyrus when processing prosocial facial affect predicted reduced levels of positive psychotic symptoms, where one individual is clearly an outlier.

It is clear that with such a small sample the authors have not attended to the presence of obvious extreme outlying data points from single individuals (indeed, reanalysis of the data [albeit imperfect after extracting data-points from the scatterplots] removing the outliers indicates that the data are uncorrelated).

Most press coverage of this study repeated the unsubstantiated claims in the press release.

Most press coverage of this study repeated the unsubstantiated claims in the press release.

Strengths and limitations

Strengths

  • The study authors did use a validated method to measure case notes symptoms
  • There were attempts to assess the long-term consequences of an intervention

Limitations

  • Small sample, multiple tests, one-tailed tests. The authors claim that “the final sample size remained adequate” and reference Friston (2012) – who it should be noted also asserts in the same paper “…if you cannot demonstrate a significant effect with sixteen subjects, it is probably not worth demonstrating”
  • No control groups for comparison: the lack of control group makes it impossible to attribute any changes to CBT rather than changes in medication or even aspects of lifestyle
  • Insufficient data on what happened in 8 years between studies apart from symptom ratings as derived from case notes (which will undoubtedly be written by many individuals with varying experience and backgrounds and in varying degrees of detail, or not as the case may be)
  • No repeated assessment of symptoms over the 8 years using the same measure (PANSS) as in the original study.
Small sample, no control, insufficient data, lack of repeated assessment: this study has a significant number of weaknesses.

Small sample, no control, insufficient data, lack of repeated assessment: this study has a significant number of weaknesses.

What does it all mean?

This study seems to have generated some media buzz with the one of the authors making bullish assertions about CBTp. It is difficult however to see what the fuss is about. How do we know if any changes over the time are even due to CBTp received 8 years earlier or the result of medication, or changes associated with age or indeed changes caused by anything else? Unfortunately, the study lacks the key ingredient of a control group; either healthy controls or participants with psychosis who received “treatment as usual”.

The study raises many questions that perhaps eluded the reviewers:

  • Why does the abstract talk about 22 participants when the follow-up data of symptoms applies to only 15 participants?
  • Why do the authors claim results are positive when they don’t meet conventional level of statistical significance?
  • Why did the authors check reliability of clinician ratings for only 8 rather than the full 15 patients?
  • Why did two raters disagree so much on what counted as remission? Why did they not minimally comment upon outlying data points when they are so evidently influential in the figures?
  • Other minor anomalies also appear to be present, e.g. how did the authors arrive at 20 degrees of freedom for the analyses in Table 1?

The lack of data on what happens during the 8 year period creates a chasm in any causal chain back to CBTp, what happened in the person’s lives, did they form close social bonds with others that changed how they reacted to people or did they take regular medication that helped?

In one sense it’s trivially true that everything we do changes brain function and unfortunately this study does not allow us to either rule CBT in or everything else out.

This study adds little to our knowledge about the safety and efficacy of CBT for psychosis.

This study adds little to our knowledge about the safety and efficacy of CBT for psychosis.

Links

Primary paper

Mason L, Peters E, Williams SC, Kumari V. (2017) Brain connectivity changes occurring following cognitive behavioural therapy for psychosis predict long-term recovery. Translational Psychiatry, 7(1), p.e1001.

Other references

Friston K. (2012) Ten ironic rules for non-statistical reviewers. Neuroimage 2012; 61: 1300–1310

Jauhar S, McKenna PJ, Radua J, Fung E, Salvador R, Laws KR. (2014) Cognitive-behavioural therapy for the symptoms of schizophrenia: systematic review and meta-analysis with examination of potential bias. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 204(1), pp.20-29.

Kumari V, Fannon D, Peters ER, Sumich AL, Premkumar P, Anilkumar AP, Andrew C, Phillips ML, Williams SC, Kuipers E. (2011) Neural changes following cognitive behaviour therapy for psychosis: a longitudinal study. Brain, p.awr154.

Mason L, Peters ER, Dima D, Williams SC, Kumari V. (2015) Cognitive behavioral therapy normalizes functional connectivity for social threat in psychosis (PDF). Schizophrenia Bulletin, p.sbv153.

Photo credits

Share on Facebook Tweet this on Twitter Share on LinkedIn Share on Google+
Mark as read
Create a personal elf note about this blog
Profile photo of Keith Laws

Keith Laws

Keith Laws is Professor of Cognitive Neuropsychology in the School of Life and Medical Sciences at the University of Hertfordshire. He completed a PhD at the Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge and is the author of over 100 papers and a recent book entitled 'Category-Specificity: Evidence for Modularity of Mind'. He is a Chartered Psychologist, an Associate Fellow of the British Psychological Society, a Fellow of the Royal Society of Arts, a Member of the Institute of Learning and Teaching and various academic organisations including the British Neuropsychological Society, British Neuropsychiatric Association, and the Experimental Psychology Society.

More posts - Website

Follow me here –

Profile photo of Samei Huda

Samei Huda

Dr Samei Huda has been working as a Consultant Psychiatrist for over 20years including a year in Australia. He also completed a Diploma in Cognitive Therapy (though he doesn’t see patients for therapy) and a Master of Science in Psychiatry. He has been working with an Early Intervention in Psychosis team since 2007. Over his career he has directly assessed, managed and treated hundreds of patients with psychosis of different types. He recommends people with an interest in psychosis to read www.EASEnet.dk (he is not affiliated with them). He has written a book evaluating the evidence for diagnosis and treatment in psychiatry called “The Medical Model in Mental Health” published by Oxford University Press. He has recently completed an MA in Philosophy of Mental Health from UCLAN.

More posts - Website

Follow me here –