Adolescence is a period of development characterised by significant biological, psychological and social changes, including the physical and hormonal transformations that puberty brings and the increased importance of peer relationships (Patton et al, 2016; Burnett Heyes et al, 2015).
Increases in risk-taking and sensation-seeking behaviour, however, also occur during adolescence, with the ability to self-regulate (in terms of being able to plan decisions and actions) only gradually improving over the course of adolescence (Steinberg et al, 2017). This feeds the stereotype of the dangerous and impulsive thrill-seeking teenager. Yet, it has been proposed that such behaviours could in fact be seen as rational and adaptive actions, when considered within the context of the adolescent fulfilling their developmental goals of navigating the turbulence of life and reaching independence (Romer et al, 2017).
Consequently, a new article recently published in Current Directions in Psychological Science by Professor Sarah-Jayne Blakemore (2018), has sought to discuss the social context within which adolescent risk-taking behaviours occur and delineate the implications of this for public health.
Increased freedom and peer influence
Although there will inevitably be individual differences in levels of risk-taking behaviour during adolescence, Blakemore points out that adolescents are typically living and acting within a context of increased freedom granted by parents and society, including being allowed and encouraged to spend more time unsupervised and take more control over their decisions. Such a context could conceivably allow risk-taking, experimentation and exploration to thrive in adolescence, as compared to childhood.
With the growing prominence of peer relationships in adolescence, Blakemore highlights that adolescence is also a time of heightened peer influence on young people’s opinions, behaviours and perceptions of their self-worth. Studies have shown that adolescents are more likely to take risks, such as in relation to driving, when they are with their peers, to engage in substance use when their peers also engage in such behaviour, and to be influenced by their peers’ opinions when rating the riskiness of particular situations. Yet, it is important to note, as Blakemore argues, that research also suggests that peer influence can operate in an adaptive manner, such as through peers discouraging each other from engaging in risky behaviours and influencing each other to engage in prosocial behaviours.
Why might adolescents be particularly susceptible to peer influence?
Blakemore offers several possible answers to this question, while acknowledging again that there will be individual differences in adolescents’ susceptibility to peer influence:
Hypersensitivity to or fear of social exclusion
For adolescents, the risk of social rejection as a consequence of not engaging in a particular behaviour might outweigh the health or legal risks associated with that behaviour.
Neurocognitive mechanisms
Findings from brain imaging studies, where adolescents are in the imagined presence of their peers, indicate that the relationship between peer influence and risk-taking behaviour could be underpinned by adolescents’ increased sensitivity to social reward (e.g. acceptance by one’s peer group) and/or changes in their levels of arousal (e.g. higher levels of embarrassment).
Fear of social evaluation
The social brain network develops during adolescence, meaning that adolescents grow in their ability to mentalise or attribute mental states and emotions to other people. Thus, adolescents may be more likely to behave in accordance with how they believe that their peers think or feel about their actions, meaning that they may be less likely to engage in a behaviour if they believe that it will be negatively socially evaluated by their peers.
Increased susceptibility of the brain to social stimuli
Rodent studies suggest that brain development during adolescence may be particularly affected by social stimuli, e.g. the presence of peers.
Implications
Blakemore concludes that while public health advertising and interventions aimed at reducing adolescents’ engagement in risky behaviours have tended to concentrate on delineating the health and legal risks of such behaviours, research on the susceptibility of adolescents to peer influence and its relation to risk-taking behaviour could perhaps usefully inform the development and focus of such endeavours. For example, an anti-smoking intervention in British schools, which was led by young people who were rated as influential by their peers, showed a significant reduction in young people’s smoking, compared to a control group who had received no such intervention (Campbell et al, 2008).
I would also suggest, drawing on Blakemore’s earlier point that adolescents are growing up in an environment that permits a certain level of freedom, autonomy and exploration, that such interventions could also usefully focus on how adolescents could engage safely in risky behaviours, assuming that adolescents will likely engage in risky behaviours at some point.
Limitations
- The factors influencing adolescents’ susceptibility to peer influence covered in this article are not, and were not intended to be, an exhaustive list. For instance, another factor that has been found to influence susceptibility to peer influence in adolescence is low-self esteem (Zimmerman et al, 1997)
- The cross-cultural validity of all of the findings and arguments conveyed in this article needs to be considered, given that the social contexts within which adolescents are growing up across the world will likely vary
- Although this is beyond the scope of this article, I also wonder about the applicability of this research to adults, in terms of explaining why adults are in general less likely to engage in risky behaviours than adolescents. For instance, are adults’ levels of susceptibility to peer influence lower than adolescents’? Is the degree to which adults may be influenced by fears about social evaluation also less? If so, why?
Links
Primary paper
Other references
Patton, G.C., Sawyer, S.M., Santelli, J.S., Ross, D.A., Afifi, R., Allen, N.B., … Viner, R.M. (2016). Our future: A Lancet commission on adolescent health and wellbeing. The Lancet, 387, 2423-2478.
Burnett Heyes, S., Jih, Y.R., Block, P., Hiu, C.F., Holmes, E.A., & Lau, J.Y. (2015). Relationship reciprocation modulates resource allocation in adolescent social networks: Developmental effects. Child Development, 86, 1489-1506.
Steinberg, L., Icenogle, G., Shulman, E.P., Breiner, K., Chein, J., Bacchini, D., … Takash, H.M. (2017). Around the world, adolescence is a time of heightened sensation seeking and immature self-regulation. Developmental Science, doi:10.1111/desc.12532 [PubMed abstract]
Romer, D., Reyna, V.F., & Satterthwhaite, T.D. (2017). Beyond stereotypes of adolescent risk-taking: Placing the adolescent brain in developmental context. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 27, 19-34.
Campbell, R., Starkey, F., Holliday, J., Audrey, S., Bloor, M., Parry-Langdon, N., … Moore, L. (2008). An informal school-based peer-led intervention for smoking prevention in adolescence (ASSIST): A cluster randomised trial. The Lancet, 371, 1595-1602.
Zimmerman, M.A., Copeland, L.A., Shope, J.T., & Dielman, T.E. (1997). A longitudinal study of self-esteem: Implications for adolescent development (PDF). Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 26, 117-141.
Photo credits
- Photo by Taylor Ann Wright on Unsplash
- Photo by Andrew Pons on Unsplash
- Photo by Bruce Dixon on Unsplash
- Photo by Victor Van Welden on Unsplash
- Photo by Eye for Ebony on Unsplash
[…] Peer influence and risk taking behaviour during adolescence […]