Investigating the link between loneliness and sleep quality in young people

6837914844_2ec2ee4ddc_o

A decrease in the quality of one’s sleep or any other sleep impairment is recognised as a symptom of multiple mental illnesses, including post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), generalised anxiety disorder (GAD) and major depression.

However, when it comes to the link between loneliness and sleep problems, depression rather curiously does not account for the relationship between these two factors (Cacioppo et al, 2002). A study published last month in Psychological Medicine entitled ‘Sleeping with one eye open: loneliness and sleep quality in young adults’ attempts to investigate whether the link is actually a direct one, with the study aiming to test a hypothesis that consists of two components:

  1. The lonelier young adults feel, the poorer their sleep quality
  2. That this effect is exacerbated by past exposure to violence

The grounding of this hypothesis lies in evolutionary biology: being a social species, our social nature is an adaptive trait that grants us safety in numbers, and perceiving that one is socially cut off gives rise to feelings of vulnerability (Cacioppo et al, 2006a). The hypothesis rests on this basis, for seeing that sleep is when we are at our most vulnerable, the state of mind of someone who believes that they are vulnerable to harm is completely juxtaposed to the notion of a peaceful night’s sleep.

Loneliness has been shown to be associated with numerous health problems and poor well-being, as has poor sleep. Including historical exposure to violence is an important addition in view of the growing evidence that traumatic experiences in early life are associated with a wide range psychological problems over time.

Sleep is our most vulnerable state.

Sleep is our most vulnerable state.

Methods

The sample

The participants were drawn from the Environmental Risk (E-Risk) Longitudinal Twin Study, which tracks the development of a birth cohort of 2,232 British children. The sample contained 56% monozygotic and 44% dizygotic twin pairs, with sex evenly distributed within zygosity (49% male). Families were recruited in a manner that was representative of the UK population with newborns in the 1990s, ensuring adequate numbers of children in disadvantaged homes and avoiding an excess of twins born to well-educated women using assisted reproduction. Each twin was assessed by two interviewers. The young people were assessed at age five, seven, ten, twelve and eighteen.

However, though this is part of a wider longitudinal study, the main exposure and outcomes in this paper are only measured cross-sectionally at age eighteen.

The twin study structure of this research was an attempt to control for the influence of genetics on the factors being investigated, which is of relevance since both loneliness (Goossens et al, 2015; Matthews et al, 2016) and sleep quality show genetic involvement (Barclay et al, 2010).

Measures

The researchers measured current feelings of loneliness at age 18 years using four items from the UCLA Loneliness Scale (ULS), version 3. I found this unusual, given the three, eight or twenty items are more typically used, having all been validated in young adults. I could not find a study specifically investigating validity of the four item version. I found an article (Hays and Dimatteo, 1987) specifically indicating that the eight item version is a better substitute for the ULS-20 (twenty item version) than the ULS-4. However, it was good to see the authors carry out a sensitivity analysis to see if the results hold with a different measure of loneliness.

Loneliness was also assessed using the interviewers’ reports, with the interviewers having to complete questions about their impressions of the participants’ personality and behaviour, with the answers used to “construct a measure of interviewer-rated loneliness”. Interviews are of course potentially quite subjective data collection techniques, so it is not clear how meaningful a ‘measure’ this is. Also, correlation between the self-report and interviewer ratings of loneliness was 0.46 (not very high).

In addition, the authors chose the Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support (MSPSS), a ‘perceived social support’ scale, as a marker of ‘objective’ social isolation. A 2016 paper that assessed different social isolation measures on the grounds of what aspects of social relationships they cover (structural or functional) and the degree of subjectivity asked of respondents, found the MSPSS to be more objective than the ULS but certainly not the most objective of measures of social support. Finding the right tools to measure the ‘right’ aspects of social relationships is a challenging business (Wang et al, 2016).

Sleep quality was measured using the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI; Buysse et al. 1989), where higher scores reflect worse sleep quality. The mean global PSQI score in the present sample was 5.39 (SD = 3.18), with five or greater being indicative of poor sleep quality. For comparison, a study by Knutson et al (2006) that assessed the stability of the PQSI using 610 38-50 year olds split by sex and race (black or white) had a mean PQSI score of 5.8 for the full sample, with only white men having a lower mean (5.15) than young people in the current study. It is known that sleep quality decreases with age, but the means are comparable overall.

The factors controlled for in the study were social isolation, symptoms of depression, anxiety, alcohol abuse and dependence, ADHD and PTSD, as well as individuals who were not in employment, education or training or who were a parent of an infant, as these are circumstances which could lead to changes in social activities and sleeping schedules. These are an appropriate set of covariates, considering we know these things have an established association with both sleep and loneliness. Furthermore, PTSD and anxiety disorders feature hypervigilance, which they have sensibly factored in, alongside PTSD being associated with previous traumatic experiences, making it important to control for. Previous work has tended to focus more on depression.

Violence victimisation was assessed between the ages of 12 and 18 years using the Juvenile Victimization Questionnaire (JVQ; Finkelhor et al. 2011), with exposure to maltreatment in childhood assessed via interviews with participants’ mothers. Information on maltreatment collected over the years of data collection was compiled into a profile for each participant. These profiles were reviewed by two clinical psychologists and coded ‘no harm’ (78.9%), ‘probable harm’ (15.4%) and ‘definite harm’ (5.7%).

Data analysis

Linear regressions were used to examine the associations between loneliness and overall sleep quality, as well as the exacerbating effect of violence victimisation on the association between loneliness and sleep quality. As both loneliness and sleep quality were measured via self-report, a sensitivity analysis was conducted by testing the association between interviewer ratings of participants’ loneliness and self-reported sleep quality.

Results

  • After controlling for all covariates, loneliness remained independently associated with
    • Poorer subjective sleep quality (odds ratio (OR) = 1.10, 95% Confidence Interval (CI) 1.03 to 1.16) and
    • Greater daytime dysfunction (OR = 1.24, 95% CI 1.17 to 1.31)
  • Among monozygotic twins, within-pair differences in loneliness were significantly associated with within-pair differences in sleep quality (β = 0.15, 95% CI 0.06 to 0.24)
  • Individuals exposed to victimisation during adolescence experienced both
    • Greater loneliness (β = 0.18, 95% CI 0.13 to 0.23) and
    • Worse sleep quality (β = 0.24, 95% CI 0.19 to 0.28) in early adulthood.
Loneliness was associated with poorer subjective sleep quality.

Loneliness was associated with poorer subjective sleep quality.

Conclusions

The conclusion reached by the authors was that:

diminished sleep quality is one of the many ways in which loneliness gets ‘under the skin’, and our findings underscore the importance of early intervention to reduce loneliness in young people, which may be the starting point for a cascade of physical health problems in later life.

Strengths

Overall, this paper makes for an interesting read, with the effect size of the findings not being that large but nonetheless robust. The study design has a number of strengths, including the following:

  • It focuses on young adults, making it a relevant and important addition to a literature base that predominantly focuses on the effects and prevalence of loneliness on the elderly, despite loneliness being especially prevalent in young adults and bearing the potential for significant long term morbidity. It also adds to the literature on the complex interrelationships between loneliness, depression and trauma.
  • The effort gone into generating a representative and well-controlled sample is particularly impressive: the sample has near equivalent proportions of males and females, controls for genetics via its design, takes care to control for several covariates but perhaps most impressively, ensures its sample is representative of the socioeconomic distribution of families in Britain.
  • The use of tests based on the Huber–White (sandwich) variance to correct for each family containing data for two individuals (resulting in non-independent observations) was another noteworthy measure taken by the researchers.

Limitations

  • It could be argued that the study is somewhat lacking in objective data. In its defence however, objective data of relevance in the field of social isolation is usually in the realms of factors such as ‘number of reciprocal social interactions’, whereas it is the participant’s perceived feelings that are of relevance in studies of this type and this is a factor that is unavoidably subjective. With regard to objective data on sleep, it would seem ideal to include it. Using objective techniques such as polysomnography on sample sizes any larger than a few dozen comes with significant financial and time-related burden. The fact people ‘report’ poor sleep does not always mean they actually lost sleep, and how these related methods of measuring sleep influence health is an important consideration.
  • Potential confounding caused by stress and the effects of personality factors, which play their own role in affecting sleep quality, were not taken into consideration.
  • I also wonder about the assumption that using monozygotic (MZ) twins necessarily holds family-wide influences constant. Yes, MZ twins are genetically identical, but they are still two separate individuals. Whilst it can be argued that they are highly likely to be treated similarly, if not the same, this most probably only applies to them up until a young age. As is later mentioned, the participants were around 18 years old, by which time they may have had quite different ‘environmental’ experiences or influences, including within the family.
  • Whilst not a limitation per se, the magnitude of effect of the main finding isn’t that large.
Great care was taken to ensure a controlled and representative sample, but why was stress left out?

Great care was taken to ensure a controlled and representative sample, but why was stress left out?

Implications for practice

In summary, this is a thoughtful, well designed study, which addresses important gaps in the existing literature. It stimulates discussion on the likely impacts trauma or loneliness may have on sleep in young people, so could make clinicians more likely to ask about it and consider how to address the issues raised. Its findings also mean that there is potential for further (e.g. longitudinal) research in this area, to establish just how big an effect loneliness has on sleep quality. If upon inspection of further evidence it appears to be a large effect, interventions targeting loneliness gain an additional avenue into improving the quality of life of young patients. The study also adds to the important evidence base on harmful lasting effects of traumatic early life experiences.

On a more personal note, I would like to point out that the effects of sleep disturbance in young people stretch beyond tiredness and decreased day productivity. Especially when combined with depression or anxiety, the sleep disturbances that lend themselves to decreased sleep quality can be a potent breeding ground for the illness itself. Many young service users will, I am sure, affirm an account of nights spent ruminating, encompassed in the negative thoughts that their illness brings them and denied the respite from these thoughts that sleep brings. Sleep is silence from a mind that seems to be out to get its host; a future intervention that addresses loneliness and perhaps by extension improves sleep quality will be of significant importance not just to those struggling with mental health problems as young adults, but also those who may be at risk of developing them.

Sleep is silence from a mind that seems to be out to get its host.

Sleep is silence from a mind that seems to be out to get its host.

Links

Primary paper

Matthews T, Danese A, Gregory A, Caspi A, Moffitt T, Arseneault L. (2017). Sleeping with one eye open: loneliness and sleep quality in young adults. Psychological Medicine, pp.1-10. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291717000629

Other references

Cacioppo JT, Hawkley LC, Berntson GG, Ernst JM, Gibbs AC, Stickgold R, Hobson J. (2002) Do lonely days invade the nights? Potential social modulation of sleep efficiency. Psychological Science 13, 384–387.

Cacioppo JT, Hawkley LC, Ernst JM, Burleson M, Berntson GG, Nouriani B, Spiegel D. (2006 a) Loneliness within a nomological net: an evolutionary perspective. Journal of Research in Personality 40, 1054–1085.

Goossens L, van Roekel E, Verhagen M, Cacioppo JT, Cacioppo S, Maes M, Boomsma DI. (2015) The genetics of loneliness: linking evolutionary theory to genome-wide genetics, epigenetics, and social science. Perspectives on Psychological Science 10, 213–226.

Matthews T, Danese A, Wertz J, Odgers C, Ambler A, Moffitt TE, Arseneault L. (2016) Social isolation, loneliness and depression in young adulthood: a behavioural genetic analysis. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 51, 339–348.

Barclay NL, Eley TC, Buysse DJ, Rijsdijk FV, Gregory AM. (2010) Genetic and environmental influences on different components of the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index and their overlap. Sleep 33, 659–668.

Hays R, DiMatteo M. (1987) A Short-Form Measure of Loneliness. Journal of Personality Assessment, 51(1), pp.69-81.

Valtorta NK, Kanaan M, Gilbody S, et al (2016) Loneliness, social isolation and social relationships: what are we measuring? A novel framework for classifying and comparing tools. BMJ Open 2016;6:e010799. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2015-010799

Knutson KL, Rathouz PJ, Yan LL, Liu K, Lauderdale DS. (2006) Stability of the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index and the Epworth Sleepiness Questionnaires over 1 year in early middle-aged adults: the CARDIA study. SLEEP-NEW YORK THEN WESTCHESTER-. 2006 Nov 1;29(11):1503.

Wang, J, Evans B-L, Giacco D, Forsythe R, Nebo C, Mann F, Johnson S (2016) Social Isolation in Mental Health: a conceptual and methodological review (PDF). School for Social Care Research, NIHR

Photo credits

Share on Facebook Tweet this on Twitter Share on LinkedIn Share on Google+
Mark as read
Create a personal elf note about this blog
Profile photo of Ahmed Al-Shihabi

Ahmed Al-Shihabi

My decision to want to go into psychiatry probably precluded me even getting into medical school - the best summary of myself this far into my budding career is a large, voluminous mass of hair and its attached, usually black-clad body running around trying to do as much psychiatry as is possible without actually being a psychiatrist (yet). I spend my spare time writing about my own experiences with mental illness (dysthymia and recurrent depressive disorder), hitting guitars in an attempt to make nice noises come out of them and listening to heavy metal and rock music (I feel like that probably explains why I used the verb 'hitting').

More posts

Follow me here –

Profile photo of Farhana Mann

Farhana Mann

Farhana is a Wellcome Trust Research Training Fellow at University College London. She is also a Specialist Registrar (ST6) in General Adult Psychiatry in North London. She studied Medicine at UCL, where she also completed a BSc in Immunology and Cell Pathology. She holds an MSc in Psychiatric Research and is a Member of the Royal College of Psychiatrists. In 2012 she was selected as a BBC Expert Woman. Her main research interest is in ‘loneliness’ and its impact on mental health. She is always keen to promote better public understanding of psychiatry and mental disorder.

More posts

Follow me here –