Social media: good and bad experiences and the impact on depression

robin-worrall-749755-unsplash

Depression is prevalent with age of onset concentrated between 18 and 43 (Kessler et al, 2005; Rhode et al, 2013). Depression is the leading cause of disability worldwide and a significant contributor to the global burden of disease (Bloom et al., 2011; WHO, 2017). The negative impacts for an individual can include under-achievement in education; under-employment; poorer quality relationships; and poorer physical health (Caruana et al., 2018; Fergusson & Woodward, 2002). Depression is also a key risk factor for self-harm and suicide (Chesney et al, 2014; Gould et al, 1996; Gould et al, 1998).

Social media use is ubiquitous; the most recent survey in the US showed that 88% of 18 to 29-year olds used social media. A large proportion of social media users engage with several platforms and they engage with these various platforms often. For example, 50% of Facebook users and Snapchat users visit the site several times a day and around 70% of younger users (18 to 24 year olds) engage with Snapchat multiple times a day (Smith & Anderson, 2018).

Much has been written about the potential for good and for harm in terms of social media use. For example, it is potentially a good source of social support. However, at the same time can be used for social comparison with the potential that perceived inadequacies are highlighted.

This study adds to the understanding of this complex issue by moving beyond measuring the impact of the amount of use of social media on issues like depression, to understanding the impact of different types of social media experiences on depression. Given people are increasingly using a range of social media platforms, the study also usefully includes a range of social media platforms.

Depression is prevalent with the potential for negative outcomes. Understanding the potential role of social media to mollify or exacerbate the risk of depression is important.

Depression is prevalent with the potential for negative outcomes. Understanding the potential role of social media to mollify or exacerbate the risk of depression is important.

Methods

This was a cross-sectional study of 1,179 full time University students in the US. Participants completed a survey asking about depression symptoms using the 4-item Patient-Reported Outcomes Measurement Information System (PROMIS) scale that asks participants how frequently in the past 7 days they had felt hopeless, worthless, helpless, or depressed with response options from 1 (never) to 5 (always) (Pilkonis et al., 2011).

They also asked participants to provide an estimate of the percentage of their social media experiences that were positive and a percentage of their experiences that were negative.

They used multivariable logistic regression to understand the relationship between positive and negative experiences and depression symptoms, controlling for sex, age and ethnicity, education, relationship status and living situation.

Participants were asked what percentage of their social media experiences were positive or negative.

Participants were asked what percentage of their social media experiences were positive or negative.

Results

The majority of the participants were female (62%) and white (72%), the mean age was 20.9 (SD 2.9; range 18 to 30); half the participants were single and half were living with friends.

  • Each 10% increase in positive experiences on social media was associated with a 4% decrease in depression symptoms, but this was not significant (Adjusted Odds Ratio 0.96 95% Confidence Interval 0.91 to 1.002)
  • Each 10% increase in negative experiences on social media was associated with a 20% increase in depression symptoms, which was significant (Adjusted Odds Ratio 1.20 95% Confidence Interval 1.11 to 1.31).

The authors tested the associations in various ways (e.g. they included in the model just positive experiences and then just negative experiences, and then both positive and negative experiences), and findings were consistent across each type of analysis that they did.

Negative experiences on social media were related to increases in depression symptoms in this group of US university students.

Negative experiences on social media were related to increases in depression symptoms in this group of US university students.

Conclusions

The authors conclude that negative experiences are more influential on depression symptoms than positive experiences, which might be to do with the tendency for people to focus more on the negative aspects of self, others and the world around us (called negativity bias).

The authors also highlight the complexity operating in terms of the types of social relationships that exist within the context of social media. They propose that people connected via social media may not have close and trusting relationships but be more like casual acquaintances and that this kind of distant relationship may increase feelings of disconnection as well as misunderstandings, resulting in negative experienes and increased depression.

The negative experiences we have on social media can have significant negative impacts on our mood.

The negative experiences we have on social media can have significant negative impacts on our mood.

Strengths and limitations

While the authors highlight that the age of the sample is relevant to the age of most users of social media, findings may not be relevant to those outside of the age range of 18-30. A study of adolescents would be an excellent extension to this study given adolescence is a period of time when there is increasing peer identification (Christie & Viner, 2005).

The authors point out that this study is cross sectional and so can’t answer questions about causality; it can’t therefore be concluded that negative experiences on social media cause depression. It might be that depression causes the negative experiences, and more likely that there is an interaction between these factors.

This study cannot tell us whether negative social media experiences actually cause depression; simply that there is an interaction between these two factors.

This study cannot tell us whether negative social media experiences actually cause depression; simply that there is an interaction between these two factors.

Implications for practice

The way the study has been done means that results can easily be implemented; clinicians need simply to add to their assessment questions: “what percentage of your experiences on social media are negative?”. They can then spend time exploring what constitutes ‘negative’ for them and how that individual might be able to avoid or deal with these negative experiences. This might be via cognitive strategies that focus on the way in which the young person is understanding the experience, as well as through behavioural strategies such as choosing not to engage in social media, especially at vulnerable times, e.g. when tired, or stressed and feeling down.

From a public health perspective, there is a need to educate people about the potential risks of using social media and about how to manage themselves and their time on social media in a way that means they are less likely to be exposed to negative experiences.

As has been pointed out in the suicide prevention field, people are using social media; we need to harness the potential for the positive while continuing to understand and mollify the risks (Robinson et al., 2015).

There is certainly further work required to understand the nuances of what constitutes positive and negative experiences in the context of social media.

It is important to help people to understand how to avoid or manage negative experiences on social media.

It is important to help people to understand how to avoid or manage negative experiences on social media.

Conflicts of interest

I have no conflicts to declare.

Links

Primary paper

Primack BA, Bisbey MA, Shensa A, et al. The association between valence of social media experiences and depressive symptoms. Depress Anxiety. 2018;1–11. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22779

Other references

Bloom DE, Cafiero ET, Jané-Llopis E. et al (2011). The Global Economic Burden of Noncommunicable Diseases. Geneva: WHO

Caruana E, Cotton SM, Farhall J, et al (2018). A Comparison of Vocational Engagement Among Young People with Psychosis, Depression and Borderline Personality Pathology. Community Mental Health Journal 54(6) 831-841.

Chesney E, Goodwin GM, Fazel S. (2014). Risks of all-cause and suicide mortality in mental disorders: a meta-review. World Psychiatry 13 153-160.

Christie D, Viner R. (2005). ABC of adolescence: adolescent development. BMJ 330(7486) 301-304.

Fergusson DM, Woodward LJ. (2002). Mental health, educational, and social role outcomes of adolescents with depression. Archives of General Psychiatry 59(3) 225-231.

Gould MS, Fisher P, Parides M. et al (1996). Psychosocial risk factors of child and adolescent completed suicide. Archives of General Psychiatry 53(12) 1155-1162.

Gould MS, King R, Greenwald S.  et al (1998). Psychopathology associated with suicidal ideation and attempts among children and adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 37(9) 915-923.

Kessler RC, Berglund P, Demler O. et al (2005). Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey replication. Archives of General Psychiatry 62 593-602.

Pilkonis PA, Choi SW, Reise SP. et al (2011). Item banks for measuring emotional distress from the Patient-Reported Outcomes Measurement Information System (PROMIS): Depression, anxiety and anger. Assessment 18(3) 263-283.

Rhode P, Lewinsohn PM, Klein DN. et al (2013). Key characteristics of major depressive disorder occurring in childhood, adolescence, emerging adulthood, and adulthood. Clinical Psychological Science 1(1) 10.1177/2167702612457599

Robinson J, Cox GR, Bailey E. et al (2015). Social media and suicide prevention: A systematic review. Early Intervention in Psychiatry 10(2) 103-121.

Smith A, Anderson M. (2018). Social Media Use in 2018. Pew Resaerch Centre: Washington, DC. http://www.pewinternet.org/2018/03/01/social-media-use-in-2018/

WHO. (2017). Depression and Other Common Mental Disorders: Global Health Estimates. Geneva: WHO

Photo credits

Share on Facebook Tweet this on Twitter Share on LinkedIn Share on Google+
Mark as read
Create a personal elf note about this blog
Profile photo of Sarah Hetrick

Sarah Hetrick

Dr Sarah Hetrick is a clinical psychologist and Associate Professor of Youth Mental Health in the Department of Psychological Medicine, University of Auckland, and an honorary Principal Fellow in the Centre of Youth Mental Health, University of Melbourne with strong collaborative working partnerships with Orygen, The National Centre of Excellence in Youth Mental Health, Melbourne, Australia. She has an international reputation in evidence synthesis, including as a senior editor for the Cochrane Common Mental Disorders Group. Building on her primary research and her reviews in the area of youth depression and suicide risk, she held an Australian National Health and Medical Research Council Training Fellowship focused on knowledge transfer and led a range of practice improvement initiatives for young people being treated for depression. She is now the co-theme lead of the Resilient Teens theme of ‘A Better Start, E Tipu E Rea’ National Science Challenge funded by the New Zealand Government’s Ministry of Business, Innovation and Employment (https://www.abetterstart.nz). This programme of work is utilising digital technology to facilitate greater access to intervention and for young people with a range of issues that are important to them. Within this, Sarah is leading work on digital interventions to assist young people to manage intense emotions, including suicidal ideation and to prevent self-harm.

More posts - Website

Follow me here –