The prospect of this book did make me happy. The idea that a group of well-respected, eminent economists would be making the case that government should focus its efforts on increasing the happiness and life satisfaction of the population, rather than just focussing on money and Gross Domestic Product, is something to warm the heart on a cold winters evening.
The book begins by quoting Thomas Jefferson “the care of human life and happiness… is the only legitimate object of good government” and the authors then set out their main purpose as to “lay out in quantitative terms what is known about the causes of well-being”.
The authors do this by undertaking a series of analyses of data from longitudinal studies of human development (mostly from the UK), and they base their conclusions predominantly on this form of evidence. The book is fairly long (336 pages plus a lot of online supplementary information) and contains a lot of tables of results of the analyses from the longitudinal studies, but there is much of interest here, even if you don’t fancy wading through a lot of results of multivariate regression modelling.
The book has three main parts really (although it isn’t divided quite like that):
- the first covering predictors of life satisfaction (mostly in adulthood),
- the second covering predictors of childhood emotional and behavioural functioning and child academic attainment,
- and the third setting out some of the economic detail and the broader summary of the authors findings.
Methods
The methods in the first two sections of the book use fairly standard statistical models to look at risk factors for life satisfaction and then risk factors for childhood emotional and behavioural functioning and academic attainment. This brings with it the relative methodological strengths and inherent limitations in observational research of this kind (more of which shortly).
The work generally uses a single question on life satisfaction (rated from 1 to 10) as the main outcome, with most of the data on adulthood taken from the British Cohort Study, and most of the data about childhood from the Children of the 90’s study (also known as ALSPAC – this is a study that I, along with many other researchers, have collaborated with in the past. I don’t see that as a particular conflict of interest, but you can judge for yourself – it does mean I’m familiar with some of the measures the authors describe and use).
Factors associated with happiness (or life satisfaction):
There are many many findings, but here are some of the key ones. There are a number of factors that are associated with happiness (or life satisfaction):
- Unemployment is unequivocally negatively associated with happiness
- Poor mental health (measured here as depressive and anxiety symptoms) is strongly associated with lower life satisfaction, and maternal depression predicts childhood emotional and behavioural problems
- Schooling seems to have a strong and enduring association with children’s emotional health
- Income has less influence than would usually be assumed (at least in the UK – internationally poverty is associated with life satisfaction, but perhaps above a certain level of income it doesn’t benefit happiness much to have more money).
Much of this will not be a surprise to most people, however having the data analysis done means these conclusions are at least drawn from data.
Limitations
As with any piece of research, there are some limitations of the methods used, and areas where the way in which findings are interpreted can be questioned. I am going to focus on only three of these here:
First, there is a circularity in the way that the potential effects of mental health on happiness are assessed. Asking someone about how happy or otherwise they are (a key component of most assessments of depression and mental health) and then asking about how satisfied they are with life leads to a certain circularity, which probably overestimates the effects of mental health on life satisfaction. Important though mental health is, this method of tackling the question is likely to lead to an underestimate of the impact of other family and societal factors. Though the book tries to manage this by measuring mental health and life satisfaction at different time-points, and controlling for other factors using regression, neither of these is likely to eliminate this effect completely.
Second, the book moves quickly from describing associations to seeming to assume that these are causal. As Professor George Davey Smith recently tweeted:
Contrary to the trite comment that "correlation doesn't imply causation", reliable correlation does indeed imply causation – just not (necessarily, indeed probably) directly between the two variables that have been correlated https://t.co/DiOk3J9mPe
— george davey smith (@mendel_random) January 16, 2018
Analyses from observational studies conducted in this way can’t really tell us much about what causes what; they can give pointers, but this is far from conclusive proof that a causes b. An example is the one used of maternal depression, where the book states that “the biggest single family determinant of a child’s wellbeing is the mental health of the mother”. Using words like determinant suggest that maternal depression is directly adversely affecting the child’s wellbeing, whereas it is likely that much of the association seen is due to other related factors.
Third, where the study looks at the association between maternal depression and child outcomes, maternal reports are used as part of the measure for the child emotional and behavioural outcome. Having the same person (the mother) contribute to reporting both her own mental health, and her child’s is likely to inflate the association seen. This is nicely demonstrated by a paper from Ringoot and colleagues (2015), and is a methodological weakness which would lead some journals to be likely to reject this part of the analysis (Sonuga-Barke, 2018).
There are other methodological and conceptual issues, like the use of different measures to assess maternal and paternal depression, and the overlooking of gender (for example as a determinant of income), which might influence the conclusions.
Implications
Some might argue that these methodological challenges don’t really matter, but I think they do when advice is given (as it is in this book) on where governments should focus their resources. The methodological biases in the analysis, and more importantly the way they are described or not described, is likely to lead to an overemphasis on individual psychological factors, and an underestimate of wider family and societal factors, such as schooling, relationships and poverty in terms of influencing life satisfaction. This could lead to policy initiatives focussed in the wrong directions.
That is not to say that many of the findings and conclusions of this work are wrong or unimportant; the focus on happiness as an important target is welcome, and the highlighting of mental health as a potentially important component of that is a good thing, in my view, however I would argue that more evidence is still needed to support the conclusions of this book.
It’s an important book and will no doubt be read widely by people in positions of influence on policy. It’s because of this potential influence, that I think it’s important that the conclusions should be phrased more tentatively than they are, and I would hope that some of the methodological issues could be considered and addressed in subsequent papers from the authors.
Links
Primary reference
Clark AE, Fleche S, Layard R, Powdthavee N, Ward G. (2018) The Origins of Happiness: The science of well-being over the life course. Princeton University Press. 2018.
Other references
Ringoot AP, Tiemeier H, Jaddoe VW, So P, Hofman A, Verhulst FC, Jansen PW. (2015) Parental depression and child well-being: young children’s self-reports helped addressing biases in parent reports. J Clin Epidemiol. 2015 Aug;68(8):928-38.
Sonuga-Barke EJS. (2018) ‘The way we do the things we do’ – decision making transparency at the Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2018 Jan;59(1):1-4.
Photo credits
- bixentro CC BY 2.0
- Photo by Justin Peterson on Unsplash
- Photo by Edu Lauton on Unsplash
- Photo by Picsea on Unsplash
Thanks for this Paul – of course attention on these issues is always welcome -your analysis of the difficulties of doing this kind of research is really important particularly because as you say it may lead to policy initiatives pointing us in completely the wrong direction ie away from the social determinants of health I also have some misgivings about the idealisation of ‘happiness’. To assume happiness is what we should aspire to seems to ignore the more tractible and closer aspirations such as having a sense of purpose, engagement and meaning in life. My own hypothesis is that it is these that may help us feel ‘happy’ or ‘satisfied’ and that aiming for happiness or satisfaction is as useful as chasing our tails or seeking the pot at the end of the rainbow,